Thinking, Fast and Slow

Thinking, Fast and Slow

by

Daniel Kahneman

Teachers and parents! Our Teacher Edition on Thinking, Fast and Slow makes teaching easy.

Thinking, Fast and Slow: Part 1, Chapter 4 Summary & Analysis

Summary
Analysis
Kahneman next introduces how we form associations and stories. He presents two words: “Bananas” and “Vomit.” He then points out that seeing those two words together causes our minds to experience some disgust, recall unpleasant memories, and form sketchy scenarios that may cause us to have temporary aversions to bananas. These associations occur quickly and effortlessly, as our System 1 attempts to make as much sense as possible of the two words.
The introduction of stories, and our automatic tendency to create them, becomes a central theme throughout the rest of Kahneman’s book. This unconscious tendency, which allows us to simplify and make more sense of the world, also disallows our understanding of statistics and our knowledge that the world is sometimes random.
Themes
Intuition, Deliberation, and Laziness Theme Icon
Stories and Subjectivity vs. Statistics and Objectivity Theme Icon
The brain is constantly building associations of ideas, with each idea linked to a vast network of other ideas. Causes are linked to effects (virus to cold); things to properties (lime to green); and things to categories (banana to fruit). The mind does not go through a sequence of ideas one at a time, but instead one idea activates many others.
The associations and categorizations that we build contribute to the stories that we formulate about the world, particularly the “causes to effects” network that Kahneman describes here.
Themes
Intuition, Deliberation, and Laziness Theme Icon
Stories and Subjectivity vs. Statistics and Objectivity Theme Icon
In the 1980s, psychologists discovered that exposure to a word causes immediate and measurable changes in a person’s ability to retrieve related words. For example, if you have recently seen or heard the word EAT, you will be more likely to complete the word fragment SO_P as SOUP. But if you have recently seen or heard the word WASH, you will be more likely to complete it as SOAP. This tendency is called priming, because the idea of EAT primes the idea of SOUP, and WASH primes SOAP. And EAT primes not only the word SOUP, but also a multitude of food-related ideas.
Kahneman explains how System 1 learns different patterns and categories; as it becomes better at intuiting different calculations, this automatic processing also opens itself to mistakes and also to being affected by different stimuli of which we may be unaware.
Themes
Intuition, Deliberation, and Laziness Theme Icon
Priming is not merely restricted to concepts and words. In an experiment conducted with students from NYU, one group of students had to unscramble sentences that contained words associated with the elderly, while another group had more neutral words. After unscrambling the sentences, each student had to walk down a hall to get to the next experiment. The students who had words that evoked the elderly walked significantly more slowly down the hall than the others. Thus, people can be primed for a behavior.
Priming serves as an example of how unaware we can be of how the things we process automatically can affect our decisions and behavior. Simply reading the words on a page—an automatic process controlled by System 1—can change our walking pace significantly.
Themes
Intuition, Deliberation, and Laziness Theme Icon
Get the entire Thinking, Fast and Slow LitChart as a printable PDF.
Thinking, Fast and Slow PDF
This link also worked in reverse. In another study with university students, students were asked to walk around a room for five minutes at a very slow pace. After this experience, the participants were much quicker to recognize words related to old age than other students who were made to walk at a normal pace.
The reverse example shows just how strong the associations that System 1 creates are, such that our own behavior can actually prime us to think differently.
Themes
Intuition, Deliberation, and Laziness Theme Icon
Gestures can also unconsciously influence our thoughts. People who are made to smile by holding a pencil between their lips find things funnier than those who are made to frown—even though they don’t realize that they are being made to do so. People who are told to shake their heads while listening to a message are less likely to accept that message than people who are told to nod their heads while listening to the same thing.
Kahneman again reinforces the automatic and subconscious nature of these System 1 associations. Even smiling or nodding without meaning to or realizing that one is doing so—merely the physical act itself—activates built-in feelings that we find things funny or that we agree with them.
Themes
Intuition, Deliberation, and Laziness Theme Icon
Studies of priming effects sometimes threaten our self-image as conscious and autonomous. A study of voting patterns in 2000 showed that support for propositions that increased school funding gained far more support when the polling station was in a school than when it was at a nearby location. This difference surprisingly outweighed the difference in voting between parents and other voters.
Kahneman expands to show how broad an effect priming can have on our subconscious. These aren’t necessarily mistakes, but they are certainly ways in which the laziness of our System 1 can cause it to be vulnerable to biases.
Themes
Intuition, Deliberation, and Laziness Theme Icon
Students who are primed for money in an experiment (even unconsciously) are more independent and selfish. They are less willing to help another student who pretends to be confused about a task. When an experimenter dropped a bunch of pencils on the floor, money-primed people will pick up fewer pencils.
This experiment has large implications for a society that focuses constantly on money. Being primed for money may lead to or explain a general bias towards individualism—and away from altruism.
Themes
Intuition, Deliberation, and Laziness Theme Icon
Kahneman writes that people often react to this information in disbelief. He tries to quell fears by saying that the effects of the primes are “robust but not necessarily large”—that among 100 voters, only a few uncertain people will vote differently on a school issue if their precinct is in a school. But the results are not made up, and they are true about everyone.
Kahneman brings up the fact that everyone is affected by these things in order to try to tamp down our inherent overconfidence. As he writes later in the book, we often like to assume the best about ourselves, even while seeing the pitfalls faced by others. This passage is meant to counteract those thoughts.
Themes
Human Fallibility and Overconfidence Theme Icon
Quotes
Kahneman concludes with an experiment conducted in an office kitchen. The office asked for people to pay for the tea or coffee that they consumed by putting money into an “honesty box” with suggested prices posted. Above the price listing was a decorative poster. Each week, they poster would shift between a flowery pattern and a set of eyes. The weeks in which there were eyes above the prices, people contributed almost three times as much money. System 1’s processing, therefore, often affects us without our even being aware of it.
This experiment perhaps brings up the true dangers of priming: the fact that people are susceptible to influence as a result of these primes. Being aware of them, and knowing that we are affected by them, perhaps gives us some more awareness of the environment around us and how we can be manipulated by it (in the same way that we can be manipulated by positive or negative framing devices).
Themes
Intuition, Deliberation, and Laziness Theme Icon